Optimism bias
WikiDoc Resources for Optimism bias |
Articles |
---|
Most recent articles on Optimism bias Most cited articles on Optimism bias |
Media |
Powerpoint slides on Optimism bias |
Evidence Based Medicine |
Clinical Trials |
Ongoing Trials on Optimism bias at Clinical Trials.gov Trial results on Optimism bias Clinical Trials on Optimism bias at Google
|
Guidelines / Policies / Govt |
US National Guidelines Clearinghouse on Optimism bias NICE Guidance on Optimism bias
|
Books |
News |
Commentary |
Definitions |
Patient Resources / Community |
Patient resources on Optimism bias Discussion groups on Optimism bias Patient Handouts on Optimism bias Directions to Hospitals Treating Optimism bias Risk calculators and risk factors for Optimism bias
|
Healthcare Provider Resources |
Causes & Risk Factors for Optimism bias |
Continuing Medical Education (CME) |
International |
|
Business |
Experimental / Informatics |
Please Take Over This Page and Apply to be Editor-In-Chief for this topic: There can be one or more than one Editor-In-Chief. You may also apply to be an Associate Editor-In-Chief of one of the subtopics below. Please mail us [1] to indicate your interest in serving either as an Editor-In-Chief of the entire topic or as an Associate Editor-In-Chief for a subtopic. Please be sure to attach your CV and or biographical sketch.
Optimism bias is the demonstrated systematic tendency for people to be over-optimistic about the outcome of planned actions. This includes over-estimating the likelihood of positive events and under-estimating the likelihood of negative events. It is one of several kinds of positive illusion to which people are generally susceptible.
Experimental demonstration
Armor and Taylor review a number of studies that have found optimism bias in different kinds of judgement.[1] These include:
- Second-year MBA students overestimated the number of job offers they would receive and their starting salary.
- Students overestimated the scores they would achieve on exams.
- Almost all the newlyweds in a US study expected their marriage to last a lifetime, even while aware of the divorce statistics.
- Professional financial analysts consistently overestimated corporate earnings.
- Most smokers believe they are less at risk of developing smoking-related diseases than others who smoke.
Students in one study rated themselves as much less likely than their peers (students of the same sex at the same college) to experience negative life events such as developing a drinking problem, having a heart attack, being fired from a job or divorcing a few years after getting married.[2]
Optimism bias does not apply universally. For example, people overestimate their chances of experiencing very low-frequency events, including negative events.
Effects of overconfidence
Example: Increased risk taking and insufficient preventive care
Optimistic overconfidence bias can induce people to underinvest in primary and preventative care and other risk reducing behaviors, like abstinence from smoking.[3]
Example: Credit card borrowing and penalty rates and fees
Overconfidence causes many individuals to grossly underestimate their odds of making a payment late. Statistically, many people are quite likely to make at least one or more payments late due to the normal range of difficulties and delays in day-to-day life. Overconfidence bias causes these individuals to grossly underestimate the odds of this happening, and therefore to accept grossly punitive fees and rates (for example an interest rate of nearly 30 %) as a result of otherwise minor transgressions like a late payment. Other companies now have extended on this approach, by increasing interest rates to punitive rates for any late payment even if it is to another creditor. Overconfidence bias makes these terms more acceptable to borrowers than if they were accurately calibrated.
Overconfidence bias also causes many individuals to substantially underestimate the probability of having serious financial or liquidity problems - for example from a sudden job loss or severe illness. This can cause individuals to take on excessive debt under the expectation that they will do "better than average" in the future and be readily able to pay it off.
Overconfidence, locus of control and depression
Overconfidence bias may cause many individuals to overestimate their degree of control as well as their odds of success. This may be protective against depression - since Seligman and Maier's model of depression includes a sense of learned helplessness and loss of predictability and control. Depressives tend to be more accurate, and less overconfident in their assessments of the probabilities of good and bad events occurring to others but they tend to overestimate the probability of bad events happening to them[citation needed]. This has caused some researchers to consider that overconfidence bias may be adaptive and/or protective in some situations.
Optimism bias and planning
Optimism bias arises in relation to estimates of costs and benefits and duration of tasks. It must be accounted for explicitly in appraisals, if these are to be realistic. Optimism bias typically results in cost overruns, benefit shortfalls, and delays, when plans are implemented.
The UK government explicitly acknowledges that optimism bias is a problem in planning and budgeting and has developed measures for how to deal with optimism bias in government (HM Treasury 2003[dead link] ). The UK Department for Transport requires project planners to use so-called "optimism bias uplifts" for large transport projects in order to arrive at accurate budgets for planned ventures (Flyvbjerg and Cowi 2004).
In a debate in Harvard Business Review, between Daniel Kahneman, Dan Lovallo, and Bent Flyvbjerg, Flyvbjerg (2003) – while acknowledging the existence of optimism bias – pointed out that what appears to be optimism bias may on closer examination be strategic misrepresentation. Planners may deliberately underestimate costs and overestimate benefits in order to get their projects approved, especially when projects are large and when organizational and political pressures are high. Kahneman and Lovallo (2003) maintained that optimism bias is the main problem.
Mechanisms
A brain-imaging study found that, when imagining negative future events, signals in the amygdala, an emotion centre of the brain, are weaker than when remembering past negative events. This weakened consideration of possible negative outcomes is one possible mechanism for optimism bias.[4]
See also
References
Further reading
|
- Pages with citations using unsupported parameters
- Pages using citations with accessdate and no URL
- All articles with unsourced statements
- Articles with unsourced statements from November 2008
- Articles with invalid date parameter in template
- All articles with dead external links
- Articles with dead external links from March 2009
- Cognitive biases