Amnesia epidemiology and demographics: Difference between revisions
Zehra Malik (talk | contribs) |
Zehra Malik (talk | contribs) |
||
Line 10: | Line 10: | ||
===Incidence=== | ===Incidence=== | ||
Worldwide, the incidence of transient global amnesia is approximately 2.9– 10 per 100,000 cases per year. <ref>Quinette P, Guillery-Girard B , Dayan J , et al. ''What does transient global amnesia really mean? Review of the literature and thorough study of 142 cases''. Brain 2006;129 (Part 7) :1640–58.</ref> | Worldwide, the incidence of transient global amnesia is approximately 2.9– 10 per 100,000 cases per year. <ref>Quinette P, Guillery-Girard B , Dayan J , et al. ''What does transient global amnesia really mean? Review of the literature and thorough study of 142 cases''. Brain 2006;129 (Part 7) :1640–58.</ref> | ||
===Gender=== | ===Gender=== | ||
Research has found that in general the earliest recollections of females with childhood amnesia are earlier and more vivid than those of males (Gleitman, et al., 2004). One study found that when 8-year old subjects were asked to recall events they had been interviewed about between 40 and 70 months old, the females provided significantly more vivid memories than the males (Fivush, et al., 1999). A similar study with adults found that women generally can recall earlier and more vivid memories than men (MacDonald, et al., 2000). It has been suggested that this pattern is due to differences in how males and females interact as children, especially the types of conversations they have (Gleitman, et al., 2004). | Research has found that in general the earliest recollections of females with childhood amnesia are earlier and more vivid than those of males (Gleitman, et al., 2004). One study found that when 8-year old subjects were asked to recall events they had been interviewed about between 40 and 70 months old, the females provided significantly more vivid memories than the males (Fivush, et al., 1999). A similar study with adults found that women generally can recall earlier and more vivid memories than men (MacDonald, et al., 2000). It has been suggested that this pattern is due to differences in how males and females interact as children, especially the types of conversations they have (Gleitman, et al., 2004). |
Revision as of 03:06, 8 March 2021
Amnesia Microchapters |
Diagnosis |
---|
Treatment |
Case Studies |
Amnesia epidemiology and demographics On the Web |
American Roentgen Ray Society Images of Amnesia epidemiology and demographics |
Risk calculators and risk factors for Amnesia epidemiology and demographics |
Editor-In-Chief: C. Michael Gibson, M.S., M.D. [1]; Associate Editor(s)-in-Chief: Jesus Rosario Hernandez, M.D. [2]
Epidemiology and Demographics
Prevalence
- The prevalence of dissociative amnesia is 1,000 to 2,600 per 100,000 (1.0% to 2.6%) of the overall population.[1]
- Prevalence of Alzheimer's disease is 20% over the age of 85.[2]
Incidence
Worldwide, the incidence of transient global amnesia is approximately 2.9– 10 per 100,000 cases per year. [3]
Gender
Research has found that in general the earliest recollections of females with childhood amnesia are earlier and more vivid than those of males (Gleitman, et al., 2004). One study found that when 8-year old subjects were asked to recall events they had been interviewed about between 40 and 70 months old, the females provided significantly more vivid memories than the males (Fivush, et al., 1999). A similar study with adults found that women generally can recall earlier and more vivid memories than men (MacDonald, et al., 2000). It has been suggested that this pattern is due to differences in how males and females interact as children, especially the types of conversations they have (Gleitman, et al., 2004).
Race
Race has also been shown to play a role in the effect of childhood amnesia. One study found that Europeans had later first retrievable memories than New Zealand Maoris, and Asians had still later ones. This suggests that the importance of the past in Maori culture may have something to do with their particularly early first retrievable memory (MacDonald, et al., 2000). It is not known definitively, however, why the racial pattern of childhood amnesia is observed.
References
- ↑ Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders : DSM-5. Washington, D.C: American Psychiatric Association. 2013. ISBN 0890425558.
- ↑ Erickson KR (1990). "Amnestic disorders. Pathophysiology and patterns of memory dysfunction". West J Med. 152 (2): 159–66. PMC 1002292. PMID 2154898.
- ↑ Quinette P, Guillery-Girard B , Dayan J , et al. What does transient global amnesia really mean? Review of the literature and thorough study of 142 cases. Brain 2006;129 (Part 7) :1640–58.